Saturday, May 29, 2010

James B. Rowe - Conversion Disorder: Understanding the Pathogenic Links between Emotion and Motor Systems in the Brain

http://farm3.static.flickr.com/2133/2423827260_4077c069a4.jpg

Interesting article - a look at understanding the neurobiological basis of conversion disorder. Hopefully, the more we can learn about this disorder, the sooner we will find some form of reliable treatment approaches. Here is a definition of conversion disorder, known as somatoform disorder in the DSM-IV-TR.

Mark Dombeck, Ph.D. Updated: Mar 27th 2002

Welcome to our Conversion Disorders topic center. The Conversion Disorders (technically known as the Somatoform Disorders) are a family of disorders whose defining characteristic is that they all involve physical bodily symptoms suggestive of a medical problem, in the absence of evidence of medical problems that could account such symptoms. Conversion disorders can co-occur with medical problems, but only when the medical problems are insufficient all by themselves to account for the severity of the conversion symptoms a given patient experiences.

Examples of conversion disorders include Hypocondriasis, Pain Disorder (including chronic pain), Conversion Disorder and Somatization Disorder (historically known as hysteria). Hypocondriasis involves a fear of contracting a serious illness which is experienced so deeply that the patient often misinterprets normal bodily experiences as evidence of their having a serious disease. Somatization Disorder and Conversion disorder both involve the experience of multiple sensory, digestive sexual and neurological type symptoms (such as temporary paralysis of an arm or leg, irritable bowel, or temporary blindness), with the collection of symptoms being more pronounced and longer lasting in Somatization Disorder as compared to Conversion. Pain disorder (particularly in its chronic form) involves the experience of pain sensations in the absence of an injury, or continuing on after the causal injury has healed.

Given that they all involve physical (and not psychological) forms of pain, it is probably not very surprising that conversion disorder patients tend to be identified most often in medical settings. Historically, physicians doubted that their conversion patients were experiencing ‘real’ symptoms because they were unable to locate a sufficient medical cause for them to be present. Those physicians that accept their patients complaints at all came to the conclusion that many conversion symptoms were best regarded as physical expressions of what was really psychological and emotional pain. In other words, a patient might be experiencing severe psychological symptoms (anxiety, depression, etc.) but either lack the ability to express these emotions directly, or be inhibited from expressing them directly for cultural or similar reasons. Admitting feelings of anxiety might be very shameful for a given patient, for example. Such patients are thought to one degree or another to ‘convert’ their emotional difficulties into bodily symptoms; taking anxiety, for instance, and expressing it as a headache, and an irritable bowel. None of this is thought to occur intentionally. To the extend that conversion occurs, it is thought to occur unconsciously, and without any manipulative intent.

Read more, starting with symptoms.

Examples of Conversion disorders are generally associated with psychological stressors and account for ~5% of neurology out-patient referrals; conversion disorder is a stable and accurate diagnosis (Perkin, 1989; Stone et al., 2005, 2009), so while in the past it was known as "hysteria," today it is a serious and not well understood medical/psychological issue.

Here is the main article under consideration.
Conversion Disorder: Understanding the Pathogenic Links between Emotion and Motor Systems in the Brain

James B. Rowe

Posted: 05/24/2010

Brain. 2010;133(5):1295-1297. © 2010 Oxford University Press

Abstract and Introduction

Introduction

When neurology and psychiatry moved apart from each other around the turn of the 20th century, casualties included the many patients with unexplained neurological disorders. Many labels have been applied to these patients. Some are descriptive (functional disorders, medically unexplained symptoms), while others refer to a presumed aetiology (psychogenic, hysteria, non-organic) or putative mechanism (dissociative or conversion disorder). Whatever is the label, for some physicians these patients are among the most interesting and challenging in the clinic. For others they are frustrating, using much time and resources for an often disappointing outcome.

Whether you are an enthusiast or nihilist, it is clear that that neither neurology nor psychiatry alone have succeeded in sufficiently advancing our neurobiological understanding or management of these disorders. The slow progress is in stark contrast to the scale of the problem. Medically unexplained neurological symptoms account for ~30% of referred neurology out-patients (Carson et al., 2000; Stone et al., 2009). Conversion disorder alone, explicitly associated with psychological stressors at the outset, accounts for ~5% of referrals and is a stable, accurate diagnosis (Perkin, 1989; Stone et al., 2005, 2009).

Psychogenic movement disorders are common in neurological practice, including tremor, dystonia, gait change and paralysis. Diagnostic criteria emphasize clinical observations such as inconsistency, distractibility and false neurological signs (Fahn and Williams, 1988). These can be both sensitive and specific, at least in the context of a movement disorder clinic (Shill and Gerber, 2006). Physiological criteria have also been developed, such as coherence of tremor oscillations distinguishing psychogenic from organic syndromes (McAuley and Rothwell, 2004). The emphasis in this 'neurological' approach is on the physical examination, supplemented by physiological tests. The psychological aspects of the disease take second place, e.g. requiring 'obvious' but not specified psychiatric or emotional disturbance (Fahn and Williams, 1988; Shill and Gerber, 2006).

In contrast, the 'psychiatric' approach emphasizes features of the clinical interview, including psychological factors at outset and inferences of intent. This is not ideal as a diagnostic 'gold standard' for developing a biological science of conversion disorder but the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) at least provides strict diagnostic criteria. The diagnosis requires the temporal association of psychological factors with the neurological symptoms (Criterion B: note that in the current revision of DSM, the aetiological relevance of the psychological factors has been downgraded from causal to association only). A further condition (Criterion C) is the lack of wilful simulation of symptoms (otherwise indicative of malingering, factitious disorders or 'Munchausen syndrome') but this is difficult to substantiate.

The neurological approach to psychogenic movement disorders and the psychiatric approach to conversion movement disorder are therefore both problematic. Many patients move between neurological and psychiatric services, but a unified formulation may not emerge, contributing to an unsatisfactory outcome. Importantly, we lack mechanisms to explain the neurological manifestation of the conversion disorder, even when likely psychological aetiological factors are established. An objective, explanatory framework for conversion disorder is clearly required, one that brings together both psychological and physiological phenomena.

Against this troubled background, in the current issue, Voon et al. (p 1526) use functional magnetic resonance imaging to study conversion movement disorders. The combination of psychiatrists and neurologists among the research team is important, and not just for the credibility of this particular study. It also signifies a trend towards a common understanding of conversion disorder, which is important for developing effective neuropsychiatric management strategies.

Voon et al. are not the first to use neuroimaging methods to study conversion disorder or psychogenic movement disorders [see Nowak and Fink (2009) for a review of 'psychogenic paralysis']. However, previous studies often used motor tasks that are close to the functional deficit. For example, in studies of conversion/psychogenic paralysis, participants have been asked to try to move the affected limb, imagine moving the paralysed limb or feign immobility of the 'good' limb (Marshall et al., 1997; Halligan et al., 2000; Spence et al., 2000; Ward et al., 2003; de Lange et al., 2007). These studies indicate abnormalities outside the core motor network, including the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex. The results support the hypothesis of abnormal inhibition of motor systems by limbic regions (Marshall et al., 1997; Halligan et al., 2000; Spence et al., 2000; Ward et al., 2003) or impairments of motor conceptualization (Burgmer et al., 2006; de Lange et al., 2007). However, imaging abnormalities identified from tasks that are based directly on the motor deficits can be confounded by differences in motor performance and sensory feedback (the latter studied directly by Vuilleumier et al., 2001). In addition, the interactions between limbic and motor areas are inferred, but have not been shown directly in the previous studies.

A different strategy has been taken by Voon et al. They study patients using a task that they can perform normally, and one that is at first sight unrelated to the motor deficit: a gender discrimination task with face pictures. By manipulating the emotional expression of the faces, irrelevant to the task at hand, they are able to study a robust neurocognitive system for emotion processing and arousal, including the amygdala. This choice of task is very important, given the emotional events that may contribute to the initiation or maintenance of conversion disorder.

A second interesting aspect of this study is the inclusion of a diverse patient population with tremor, gait disorder, dystonia or mixed syndromes. This contrasts with smaller series (median n = 4) of closely matched patients in previous neuroimaging studies (Marshall et al., 1997; Halligan et al., 2000; Spence et al., 2000; Ward et al., 2003; de Lange et al., 2007). Whereas clinical heterogeneity can reduce sensitivity to the neural correlates of individual phenotypes, it also means that results reflect the communalities, not differences, of conversion movement disorders. It is all the more interesting, therefore, that common abnormalities are seen in the amygdala and its interaction with the supplementary motor area. This provides direct support for a model of conversion disorder based on abnormal limbic–motor interactions.

Is it sufficient to identify neural correlates of chronic conversion movement disorder? Several issues arise here. First, one must acknowledge that, in isolation, physiological correlations do not provide the cause or mechanisms of disease. However, they do support a change in the direction of research, striving for a neurobiological model. This should not discard the importance of psychological or psychodynamic factors, but they must lie within an evidence-based functional and causal (mechanistic) neuroanatomical model. Second, the search for a neural signature of conversion disorder implicitly challenges the historical 'non-organic' model. This problem is not unique to conversion disorder, but applies to other major psychiatric syndromes including depression or schizophrenia.

The observed neural correlates of conversion disorder might also be due to late epiphenomena or comorbidities in chronically disabled patients, e.g. depression. In the study by Voon et al., the patients do not meet diagnostic criteria for major affective or psychotic disorders or substance misuse. However, they do have significantly higher rates of depression and anxiety symptoms than controls. In separate studies, depression, remitted depression or elevated depressive symptoms without depression have been shown to influence the activity of the amygdala (Drevets, 2000) and its response to salient stimuli (Taylor Tavares et al., 2008; Beesdo et al., 2009). Voon et al. argue cogently that their findings are independent of depressive or anxiety symptoms. However, they rely partly on a lack of significant correlation between depression scores and amygdala activity (possible risk of type II error) and partly on interpretation of left versus right differences (possible risk of false lateralization arising in thresholded images). Nonetheless, the depression scores in their patients may have been elevated by somatic symptoms, a common feature of conversion disorder, rather than mood disturbance per se.

A third important aspect of the study by Voon et al. is their analysis of the connectivity of the amygdala. It has previously been proposed that conversion disorder arises from abnormal interactions between limbic and motor regions. To test hypotheses of interacting neural systems calls for formal analyses of network connectivity, but this has been lacking in previous studies. Voon et al. use two methods to study connectivity. The first is psychophysiological interactions (Friston et al., 1997), revealing that patients viewing salient stimuli have abnormal connectivity between the amygdala and the supplementary motor area. The second method, Granger Causality Modelling (Roebroeck et al., 2009), is used to identify the directionality of this effect: amygdala activity is predictive of future changes in the supplementary motor area, but not vice versa. Future studies will no doubt refine task designs and analysis options to include more realistic biophysical models with anatomically defined networks (Friston, 2009). As a first step, however, the concordance between connectivity methods and their immediate relevance to the predicted limbic–motor interactions is impressive.

There remain many unanswered questions. For example, firstly, why do even common and mild mood disorders or stressors lead to bizarre and disabling conversion disorders/psychogenic movement disorders? Secondary gain, or exposure to neurological illness in personal or professional life, may be relevant (Shill and Gerber, 2006), but they lack objective measures and are prone to recollection bias. Secondly, what determines the nature of the neurological deficit—tremor, paralysis, blindness, sensory loss? Here, at least the approach taken by Voon et al. provides clear testable hypotheses regarding interactions between amygdala and visual or sensory cortex in other syndromes. Thirdly, why do patients experience a sense of loss of control over their movements? This speaks to the broader neuroscience of volition (Haggard, 2008) and disturbed sense of 'agency' in neurological disorders (Moore et al., 2009).

Answers to these questions will require further long-term integration of psychiatry and neurology during training, in the clinic and in the laboratory. With its high prevalence and disability, conversion disorder clearly requires higher priority for research, into both basic mechanisms and effective therapies. In the meantime, be inspired by the work of Voon et al., whose results are a significant step towards understanding the neurobiological basis of conversion disorder and developing better therapies.


1 comment:

Increase Biceps said...

Coversion disorder in my opinion is a rather shady thing, i ready though the article to clear the doubs and it was a nice article, thanks.